Menus Subscribe Search

Follow us


Findings

van-gogh-wheat

Researchers looked at how viewers of artwork were influenced when they knew it had been done by someone whose name they recognized as famous. (Photo: Public Domain)

What, Me Biased?

• February 26, 2014 • 4:00 AM

Researchers looked at how viewers of artwork were influenced when they knew it had been done by someone whose name they recognized as famous. (Photo: Public Domain)

New research finds we retain our unwarranted faith in our capacity to be objective, even in the face of obvious evidence to the contrary.

Pretty much all of us are prone to “bias blindness.” We can easily spot prejudice in others, but we’re oblivious to our own, insisting on our impartiality in spite of any and all evidence to the contrary.

Newly published research suggests this problem is actually worse than we thought. It finds that even when people use an evaluation strategy they concede is biased, they continue to insist their judgments are objective.

“Recognizing one’s bias is a critical first step in trying to correct for it,” writes a research team led by Emily Pronin and Katherine Hansen of Princeton University. “These experiments make clear how difficult that first step can be to reach.”

“Even when people acknowledge that what they are about to do is biased, they still are inclined to see their resulting decisions as objective.”

Although their findings have clear implications regarding political opinions, the researchers avoided such fraught topics and focused on art. In two experiments, participants (74 Princeton undergraduates in the first, 85 adults recruited online in the second) looked at a series of 80 paintings and rated the artistic merit of each on a one-to-nine scale.

Half of the participants were instructed to note the name of the artist (which was flashed onto the screen when they pushed a specific button) before making their evaluation. The others evaluated the works without knowing who painted them.

When that button was pushed, half of the paintings were identified as the product of a famous artist (usually the one who actually created the work). The others were identified as a work by an unknown artist; researchers “consulted a phone book to obtain names to assign to those paintings.”

The students who saw these names conceded that the format lent itself to bias. But they “did not view their own evaluations as any less objective than did participants in the explicitly objective condition,” the researchers note.

Not surprisingly, their evaluations were, in fact, biased by the information: They rated the merit of painters attributed to great artists higher than those works purportedly created by unknowns. On the other hand, those who did not see the alleged names of the artists “rated the artistic merit of the two groups of paintings the same,” the researchers write.

For the online experiment, the researchers added some extra precautions, explicitly pointing out that looking at the artists’ names could create bias, “in that paintings by famous painters could be rated more highly, regardless of their actual quality.”

Nevertheless, the results were identical to the first experiment. Indeed, in spite of the warning, those who saw the painters’ names “became yet more convinced of their objectivity.”

The results add to the evidence that “people have difficulty recognizing their own biases,” the researchers conclude. “It shows that even when people acknowledge that what they are about to do is biased, they still are inclined to see their resulting decisions as objective.”

This false sense of fairness and impartiality can lead all to sorts of problems. To cite just one, the researchers note that a juror who is certain he or she won’t take into account testimony ruled inadmissible may, in fact, be swayed by it.

So what’s the answer here? Pronin and her colleagues argue that the best strategy is presenting information in such a way that prevents bias. They point to the tradition of orchestras having potential members audition from behind a screen, so that they are judged solely on their talent, as opposed to their race, gender, age, or any other extraneous factors.

The researchers note that such strategies are effective, but we’re reluctant to use them because we have such strong confidence in our own objectivity. Their work provides new evidence that such faith is, sadly, unwarranted.

Tom Jacobs
Staff writer Tom Jacobs is a veteran journalist with more than 20 years experience at daily newspapers. He has served as a staff writer for The Los Angeles Daily News and the Santa Barbara News-Press. His work has also appeared in The Los Angeles Times, Chicago Tribune, and Ventura County Star.

More From Tom Jacobs

A weekly roundup of the best of Pacific Standard and PSmag.com, delivered straight to your inbox.

Recent Posts

September 30 • 4:00 AM

Grad School’s Mental Health Problem

Navigating the emotional stress of doctoral programs in a down market.


September 29 • 1:21 PM

Conference Call: Free Will Conference


September 29 • 12:00 PM

How Copyright Law Protects Art From Criticism

A case for allowing the copyright on Gone With the Wind to expire.


September 29 • 10:00 AM

Should We Be Told Who Funds Political Attack Ads?

On the value of campaign finance disclosure.


September 29 • 8:00 AM

Searching for a Man Named Penis

A quest to track down a real Penis proves difficult.


September 29 • 6:00 AM

Why Do So Many People Watch HGTV?

The same reason so many people watch NCIS or Law and Order: It’s all a procedural.


September 29 • 4:00 AM

The Link Between Depression and Terrorism

A new study from the United Kingdom finds a connection between depression and radicalization.


September 26 • 4:00 PM

Fast Track to a Spill?

Oil pipeline projects across America are speeding forward without environmental review.


September 26 • 2:00 PM

Why Liberals Love the Disease Theory of Addiction, by a Liberal Who Hates It

The disease model is convenient to liberals because it spares them having to say negative things about poor communities. But this conception of addiction harms the very people we wish to help.


September 26 • 1:21 PM

Race, Trust, and Split-Second Judgments


September 26 • 9:47 AM

Dopamine Might Be Behind Impulsive Behavior

A monkey study suggests the brain chemical makes what’s new and different more attractive.


September 26 • 8:00 AM

A Letter Becomes a Book Becomes a Play

Sarah Ruhl’s Dear Elizabeth: A Play in Letters From Elizabeth Bishop to Robert Lowell and Back Again takes 900 pages of correspondence between the two poets and turns them into an on-stage performance.


September 26 • 7:00 AM

Sonic Hedgehog, DICER, and the Problem With Naming Genes

Wait, why is there a Pokemon gene?


September 26 • 6:00 AM

Sounds Like the Blues

At a music-licensing firm, any situation can become nostalgic, romantic, or adventurous, given the right background sounds.


September 26 • 5:00 AM

The Dark Side of Empathy

New research finds the much-lauded feeling of identification with another person’s emotions can lead to unwarranted aggressive behavior.



September 25 • 4:00 PM

Forging a New Path: Working to Build the Perfect Wildlife Corridor

When it comes to designing wildlife corridors, our most brilliant analytical minds are still no match for Mother Nature. But we’re getting there.


September 25 • 2:00 PM

Fashion as a Inescapable Institution

Like it or not, fashion is an institution because we can no longer feasibly make our own clothes.


September 25 • 12:00 PM

The Fake Birth Mothers Who Bilk Couples Out of Their Cash by Promising Future Babies

Another group that’s especially vulnerable to scams and fraud is that made up of those who are desperate to adopt a child.


September 25 • 10:03 AM

The Way We QuickType


September 25 • 10:00 AM

There’s a Name for Why You Feel Obligated to Upgrade All of Your Furniture to Match

And it’s called the Diderot effect.


September 25 • 9:19 AM

School Counselors Do More Than You’d Think

Adding just one counselor to a school has an enormous impact on discipline and test scores, according to a new study.


September 25 • 9:05 AM

Sponsors: Coming to a Sports Jersey Near You

And really, it’s not that big of a deal.


September 25 • 8:00 AM

The Most Pointless Ferry in Maryland

Most of the some 200 ferries that operate in the United States serve a specific, essential purpose—but not the one that runs across the Tred Avon River.


September 25 • 7:00 AM

Hating Happiness

People all over the world are afraid of happiness, and that’s not necessarily a bad thing. It’s yet another challenge to the notion that positive thinking can heal all wounds.


Follow us


Dopamine Might Be Behind Impulsive Behavior

A monkey study suggests the brain chemical makes what's new and different more attractive.

School Counselors Do More Than You’d Think

Adding just one counselor to a school has an enormous impact on discipline and test scores, according to a new study.

How a Second Language Trains Your Brain for Math

Second languages strengthen the brain's executive control circuits, with benefits beyond words.

Would You Rather Go Blind or Lose Your Mind?

Americans consistently fear blindness, but how they compare it to other ailments varies across racial lines.

On the Hunt for Fake Facebook Likes

A new study finds ways to uncover Facebook Like farms.

The Big One

One company, Amazon, controls 67 percent of the e-book market in the United States—down from 90 percent five years ago. September/October 2014 new-big-one-5

Copyright © 2014 by Pacific Standard and The Miller-McCune Center for Research, Media, and Public Policy. All Rights Reserved.